当前位置: 首页 > 雅思考试答案 > 正文

雅思听力部分 Section 1  村庄艺术展(新题) Section 2  旅游经理指导员工工作(新题) S […]

老烤鸭雅思口语限量答案请联系小助手微信号:laokaoyaielts

雅思听力部分

Section 1  村庄艺术展(新题)

Section 2  旅游经理指导员工工作(新题)

Section 3 Changing Subjects

1. A score for creadits

2. Why consult her family member to discuss the decision: A can give her good advice

3. The man’s attitude towards sport course: A good reputation

4. She agrees with which factors she need to concern more when change a major: B make up lessons

5. A little practical/B route to job

6. Information about the new course: consult the tutor

7. Notice the finance office

8. Email: welfare director

9. Contact administration office

10. Please go and contact the residence hall

 

Section 4 Development Studies: Cocoa

11. the beans were sometimes used as money

12. 16th century: Europeans added sugar to their drinking chocolate

13. 19th century: discovery of how to make solid chocolate using a press

14. Advertisers tend to emphasize its impact on people’s mood

15. Italy imports the largest amount of cocoa in the world

16. cocoa industry is important for the economy of West Africa

17. demand for West African cocoa is rising due to its lower labor cost

18. plantations vary in size while the methods are similar

19. cocoa pods are harvested and stored in stone tanks

20. cocoa factories are built near the coast for good transport

雅思阅读部分

Passage 1: Saving a forgotten forest: the longleaf pine 

 

Found only in the Deep South of America, longleaf pine woodlands have dwindled to about 3 percent of their former range, but new efforts are under way to restore them.

THE BEAUTY AND THE BIODIVERSITY of the longleaf pine forest are well-kept secrets, even in :its native South. Yet it is among the richest ecosystems in North America, rivaling tall grass prairies and the ancient forests of the Pacific Northwest in the number of spedes it shelters. And like : those two other disappearing wildlife habitats, longleaf is also critically endangered.

 

In longleaf pine forests, trees grow widely scattered, creating an open, parklike environment, more like a savanna than a forest. The trees are not so dense as to block the sun. This openness r creates a forest floor that is among the most diverse in the world, where plants such as many-flowered grass pinks, trumpet pitcher plants, Venus flytraps, lavender ladies and pineland bog- ! buttons grow. As many as 50 different spedes of wildflowers, shrubs, grasses and ferns have been I cataloged in just a single square meter.

 

Once, nearly 92 million acres of longleaf forest flourished from Virginia to Texas, the only place in the world where it is found. By the turn of the 21st century, however, virtually all of it had i been logged, paved or farmed into oblivion. Only about 3 percent of the original range still supports longleaf forest, and only about 10,000 acres of that is uncut old-growth-the rest is forest j that has regrown after cutting. An estimated 100,000 of those acres are still vanishing every year. ! However, a quiet movement to reverse this trend is rippling across the region. Governments, i private organisations (including NWF) and individual conservationists are looking for ways to ; protect and preserve the remaining longleaf and to plant new forests for future generations.

 

Figuring out how to bring back the piney woods also will allow biologists to help the plants ; and animals that depend on this habitat. Nearly two-thirds of the declining threatened or I endangered spedes in the southeastern United States are associated with longleaf. The outright destruction of longleaf is only part of their story, says Mark Danaher, the biologist for South Carovlina’s Francis Marion National Forest. He says the demise of these animab and plants also is tied j to a lack of fire, which once swept through the southern forests on a regular basis. “Fire is absolutely critical for this ecosystem and for the species that depend on it,” says Danaher.

 

Name just about any spedes that occurs in longleaf and you can find a connection to fire. Bach- j man’s sparrow is a secretive bird with a beautiful song that echoes across the longleaf flatwoods.It tucks its nest on the ground beneath clumps of wiregrass and little bluestem in the open under-story. But once fire has been absent for several years, and a tangle of shrubs starts to grow, the sparrows disappear Gopher tortoises, the only native land tortoises east of the Mississippi, are also abundant in longleaf. A keystone species for these forests, its burrows provide homes and safety to more than 300 species of vertebrates and invertebrates ranging from eastern diamond- back rattlesnakes to gopher frogs. If fire is suppressed, however, the tortoises are choked out. “If we lose fire”, says Bob Mitchell, an ecologist at the Jones Center, “we lose wildlife.”

 

Without fire, we also lose longleaf. Fire knocks back the oaks and other hardwoods that can grow up to overwhelm longleaf forests. They are fire forests,” Mitchell says. “They evolved in the lightning capital of the eastern United States.”And it wasn’t only lightning strikes that set the forest aflame. ‘”Native Americans also lit fires to keep the forest open,” Mitchell says. “So did the early pioneers. They helped create the longleaf pine forests that we know today.”

 

Fire also changes how nutrients flow throughout longleaf ecosystems, in ways we are just beginning to understand. For example, researchers have discovered that frequent fires provide extTa calcium, which is critical for egg production, to endangered red-cockaded woodpeckers. Frances James, a retired avian ecologist from Florida State University, has studied these small black- and-white birds for more than two decades in Florida’s sprawling Apalachicola National Forest. When she realised female woodpeckers laid larger clutches in the first breeding season after their territories were burned, she and her colleagues went searching for answers. wWe learned caldum is stashed away in woody shrubs when the forest is not burned/ James says. wBut when there is a fire, a pulse of calcium moves down into the soil and up into the longleaf. Eventually, this calcium makes its way up the food chain to a tree-dwelling species of ant, which is the red- cockaded’s favorite food. The result: more caldum for the birds, which leads to more eggs, more young and more woodpeckers.

 

Today, fire is used as a vital management tool for preserving both longleaf and its wildlife. Most of these fires are prescribed burns, deliberately set with a drip torch. Although the public often opposes any type of fire–and the smoke that goes with it–these frequent, low-intensity bums reduce the risk of catastrophic conflagrations. wForests are going to bum/ says Amadou Diop, NWF’s southern forests restoration manager. It‘s just a question of when. With prescribed bums, we can pick the time and the placed.”

Diop is spearheading a new NWF effort to restore longleaf. “‘It’s a species we need to go back to/ he says. Educating landowners about the advantages of growing longleaf is part of the program, he adds, which will soon be under way in nine southern states. “Right now, most longleaf is on public land,” says Jerry McCollum, president of the Georgia Wildlife Federation. “Private land is where we need to work,” he adds, pointing out that more than 90 percent of the acreage within the historic range of longleaf falls under this category.

 

Interest among private landowners is growing throughout the South, but restoring longleaf is not an easy task. The herbaceous layer–the understory of wiregrasses and other plants–also needs to be re-created. In areas where the land has not been chewed up by farming, but converted to loblolly or slash pine plantations, the seed bank of the longleaf forest usually remains viable beneath the soil. In time, this original vegetation can be coaxed back. Where agriculture has destroyed the seeds, however, wiregrass must be replanted. Right now, the expense is prohibitive, but researchers are searching for low-cost solutions.

 

Bringing back longleaf is not for the short-sighted, however. Few of us will be alive when the pines being planted today become mature forests in 70 to 80 years. But that is not stopping longleaf enthusiasts. Today,it’s getting hard to find longleaf seedlings to buy,” one of the private landowners says. “Everyone wants them. Longleaf is in a resurgence.”

 

Questions 1-5

Complete the notes below.

Choose NO MORE THAN TWO WORDS from the passage for each answer Write your answers in boxes 1-6 on your answer sheet.

Forest fire ensures that:

• Birds can locate their 1____ in the ground.

• The burrows of a species of 2____ provide homes to many other animals.

• Hardwoods such as 3____don’t take over.

• Apart from flres lit by lightning 4___:

• Fires are created by 5___ and settlers.

• Fires deliberately lit are called 6 ___.

Questions 7-9

Complete the flow-chart below.

Choose ONE WORD ONLYfrom the passage for each answer.

Write your answers in boxes 6-9 on your answer sheet.

How to increase the number of cockaded woodpeckers

Calcium stored in shrubs

Shrubs are burned

Calcium released into 7____

Travel up to the leaves

8 ___are eaten

Number of 9___ increases

More cockaded woodpeckers

 

Questions 10-13

Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 1?

In boxes 10-13 on your answer sheet, write

TRUE if the statement agrees with the information

FALSE if the statement contradicts the information

NOT GIVEN if there is no information on this

10 The sparse distribution of longleaf pine trees leads to the most diversity of species.

11 It is easier to restore forests converted to farms than forests converted to plantations.

12 The cost to restore forest is increasing recently.

13 Few can live to see the replanted forest reach its maturity.

 

Passage 2: 计算机的发展

 

Passage 3: What do Babies Know

 

As Daniel Haworth is settled into a high chair and wheeled behind a black screen, a sudden look of worry furrows his 9-month-old brow. His dark blue eyes dart left and right in search of the familiar reassurance of his mother’s face. She calls his name and makes soothing noises, but Daniel senses something

unusual is happening. He sucks his fingers for comfort, but, finding no solace, his month crumples, his body stiffens, and he lets rip an almighty shriek of distress. This is the usual expression when babies are left alone or abandoned. Mom picks him up, reassures him, and two minutes later, a chortling and alert Daniel returns to the darkened booth behind the screen and submits himself to baby lab, a unit set up in 2005 at the University of Manchester in northwest England to investigate how babies think.

 

Watching infants piece life together, seeing their senses, emotions and motor skills take shape, is a source of mystery and endless fascination—at least to parents and developmental psychologists. We can decode their signals of distress or read a million messages into their first smile. But how much do we really know about what’s going on behind those wide, innocent eyes? How much of their understanding of and response to the world comes preloaded at birth? How much is built from scratch by experience? Such are the questions being explored at baby lab. Though the facility is just 18 months old and has tested only 100 infants, it’s already challenging current thinking on what babies know and how they come to know it.

 

Daniel is now engrossed in watching video clips of a red toy train on a circular track. The train disappears into a tunnel and emerges on the other side. A hidden device above the screen is tracking Daniel’s eyes as they follow the train and measuring the diametre of his pupils 50 times a second. As the child gets bored—or “habituated”, as psychologists call the process— his attention level steadily drops. But it picks up a little whenever some novelty is introduced. The train might be green, or it might be blue. And sometimes an impossible thing happens— the train goes into the tunnel one color and comes out another.

 

Variations of experiments like this one, examining infant attention, have been a standard tool of developmental psychology ever since the Swiss pioneer of the field, Jean Piaget, started experimenting on his children in the 1920s. Piaget’s work led him to conclude that infants younger than 9 months have no innate knowledge of how the world works or any sense of “object permanence” (that people and things still exist even when they’re not seen). Instead, babies must gradually construct this knowledge from experience. Piaget’s “constructivist” theories were massively influential on postwar educators and psychologist, but over the past 20 years or so they have been largely set aside by a new generation of “nativist” psychologists and cognitive scientists whose more sophisticated experiments led them to theorise that infants arrive already equipped with some knowledge of the physical world and even rudimentary programming for math and language. Baby lab director Sylvain Sirois has been putting these smart-baby theories through a rigorous set of tests. His conclusions so far tend to be more Piagetian: “Babies,” he says, “know nothing.”

 

What Sirois and his postgraduate assistant Lain Jackson are challenging is the interpretation of a variety of classic experiments begun in the mid-1980s in which babies were shown physical events that appeared to violate such basic concepts as gravity, solidity and contiguity. In one such experiment, by University of Illinois psychologist Renee Baillargeon, a hinged wooden panel appeared to pass right through a box. Baillargeon and M.I.T’s Elizabeth Spelke found that babies as young as 3 1/2 months would reliably look longer at the impossible event than at the normal one. Their conclusion: babies have enough built-in knowledge to recognise that something is wrong.

 

Sirois does not take issue with the way these experiments were conducted. “The methods are correct and replicable,” he says, “it’s the interpretation that’s the problem.” In a critical review to be published in the forthcoming issue of the European Journal of Developmental Psychology, he and Jackson pour cold water over recent experiments that claim to have observed innate or precocious social cognition skills in infants. His own experiments indicate that a baby’s fascination with physically impossible events merely reflects a response to stimuli that are novel. Data from the eye tracker and the measurement of the pupils (which widen in response to arousal or interest) show that impossible events involving familiar objects are no more interesting than possible events involving novel objects. In other words, when Daniel

had seen the red train come out of the tunnel green a few times, he gets as bored as when it stays the same color. The mistake of previous research, says Sirois, has been to leap to the conclusion that infants can understand the concept of impossibility from the mere fact that they are able to perceive some novelty in it. “The real explanation is boring,” he says.

 

So how do babies bridge the gap between knowing squat and drawing triangles —a task Daniel’s sister Lois, 2 1/2, is happily tackling as she waits for her brother? “Babies have to learn everything, but as Piaget was saying, they start with a few primitive reflexes that get things going,” said Sirois. For example, hardwired in the brain is an instinct that draws a baby’s eyes to a human face. From brain imaging studies we also know that the brain has some sort of visual buffer that continues to represent objects after they have been removed—a lingering perception rather than conceptual understanding. So when babies encounter novel or unexpected events, Sirois explains, “there’s a mismatch between the buffer and the information they’re getting at that moment. And what you do when you’ve got a mismatch is you try to clear the buffer. And that takes attention.” So learning, says Sirois, is essentially the laborious business of resolving mismatches. “The thing is, you can do a lot of it with this wet sticky thing called a brain. It’s a fantastic, statistical-learning machine”. Daniel, exams ended, picks up a plastic tiger and, chewing thoughtfully upon its heat, smiles as if to agree.

雅思口语部分

参阅 2018年雅思口语答案

雅思写作部分

小作文:表格+柱图

大作文:In many places the roles of mothers and fathers are changing. Do you think it is a positive or negative development?

老烤鸭雅思公众号
本文固定链接: http://www.laokaoya.com/7780.html | 老烤鸭雅思-专注雅思备考

2018年10月20日亚太卷考试答案:目前有2 条评论

  1. 0楼
    Cindy

    香港考卷聽力(考和這裡寫的不一樣…)
    S1丈夫為太太慶祝晉昇預約party(有信用卡16位數字填寫)
    S2介紹一個音樂節(全部配對)
    S3關於一個幫助兒童的組織的介紹(前6題選擇,后4個填空)
    S4介紹一位將垃圾作為創作材料的藝術家

    2018-10-21 21:51 [回复]
    • Neal

      谢谢回忆,帮了小编大忙了

      2018-10-21 23:45 [回复]

发表评论

快捷键:Ctrl+Enter
error: Alert: Content is protected !!